The June 2012 issue of the magazine Adoption Today is available for free at http://bluetoad.com/publication/?i=113582.
(It’s supposed to be available at the magazine web site, too, but I wasn’t able
to make the pages turn when I went there.) It’s worth looking at under a strong
light-- in a discouraging kind of way.
This issue contains no articles by people with
advanced professional research backgrounds, or by anyone usually considered to
be an outstanding researcher or clinician in adoption or related fields. (It
has an advertisement by someone who has claimed his methods are evidence-based,
but that’s a whole ‘nother story.) That situation may not be too surprising in
a publication that’s aimed at adoptive parents, of course—but it is surprising
to see that many of the articles are by leading members of the Association for
Treatment and Training of Attachment in Children (ATTACh), an organization that
has its own belief system about attachment and has shown little interest in the
realities as demonstrated by systematic investigation. Those who doubt this
statement may want to go to www.attach.org/signssymp.htm
and look at the list of behaviors that are claimed to be indicative of
disorders of attachment; compare those with the actual definition of Reactive
Attachment Disorder in DSM-IV-Tr, and note the absence of any other “attachment
disorder” in that volume, and you’ll see what I mean.
Adoption
Today, at least in this issue, is a vehicle for
advertisement of the books, videos, and services offered by a small number of
practitioners. But it also includes articles under the bylines of many of the
same people, and some of these deserve closer attention as they reveal how the
authors follow the “party line” rather than evidence-based material about either
attachment or adoption. Incidentally, the last page of the issue contains a “CEU
quiz” for pre-adoption preparation, although it acknowledges that most states
have no requirement for this type of training.
Starting on p. 14, an article by Lark Eshleman, who I
believe is an educational psychologist, discusses attachment in terms borrowed
from ATTACh. (Eshleman apparently worked with the Craver family who were
recently convicted in the death of their adopted child, as I discussed at http://childmyths.blogspot.com/2011/11/nathaniel-craver-case-many.html.)
In her article, Eshleman provides a definition of attachment quoted from
ATTACh: “Attachment is a reciprocal process by which an emotional connection
develops between an infant and his or her primary caregiver.” Let’s give this
some consideration.
First, is attachment actually a reciprocal process? Its foundations are in reciprocal behaviors, but attachment itself is not reciprocal. Attachment is the name given to a motivational change in a young
child, one which leads him or her to show preferences for familiar people (not
just the primary caregiver, by the way). Attachment is shown most often as an
attitude, or readiness to behave, in which social preferences are acted upon
when threat or discomfort is present, not all the time.
To say that attachment is a reciprocal process implies mutuality and suggests that the caregiver is experiencing the same motivational changes as the
child, which is certainly not the case; if it were, mothers with headaches
would rush to be with their children, rather than hoping for some quiet time
alone. If the process were reciprocal, there would be no sad cases of young
children grieving over their removal from the homes of neglectful or abusive
parents who show little sign of caring about the kids.
It is true that attachment develops in the context
of social interactions with other people. But infants are so ready to form
attachments that they need relatively little encouragement or reciprocity from adults to do
so-- in fact, many adults would much
rather that their toddlers showed less attachment behavior, and rather than
encouraging the behavior may actively discourage it (but without successfully altering
it).
It’s also true that adult caregiving and infant bids
for care (the usual context of attachment development) are reciprocal. Students of parental behavior used to use the terms
epimeletic (meaning caregiving) and etepimeletic (meaning soliciting
caregiving) to emphasize the interactions between these two types of behavior.
And most social interactions take place in the caregiving situation, so attachment
is developed in association with reciprocal actions-- but that is not the same as saying attachment
is reciprocal. When the claim is made that attachment is reciprocal, it’s easy
for adoptive parents to feel that “something is wrong” when an adopted child
does not share the parents’ excitement, pleasure, and love.
Incidentally, the focus of the ATTACh definition on the "primary caregiver" is problematic. Attachment to several familiar people (even those who are sociable but not caregivers) is a feature of toddlers' lives. We can see this because toddlers can use people other than the primary caregiver as secure bases for exploration and also can turn to those people when threatened or uncomfortable. The ATTACh approach turns on the theory that the mother is absolutely central to the child's emotional development, as originally assumed by Bowlby, later dropped by Bowlby, and shown to be unlikely in this age of LGBT etc. families.
Incidentally, the focus of the ATTACh definition on the "primary caregiver" is problematic. Attachment to several familiar people (even those who are sociable but not caregivers) is a feature of toddlers' lives. We can see this because toddlers can use people other than the primary caregiver as secure bases for exploration and also can turn to those people when threatened or uncomfortable. The ATTACh approach turns on the theory that the mother is absolutely central to the child's emotional development, as originally assumed by Bowlby, later dropped by Bowlby, and shown to be unlikely in this age of LGBT etc. families.
So Eshleman is pushing a concept created by ATTACh
which is in fact not in line with the realities of attachment as systematic
research has shown them to be, and Adoption
Today is presenting this idea as reliable thinking about attachment. What
else can we find in this issue, other than advertising for Eshleman’s services
and publications? Ah—let’s look at this article by Mershona Parshall, starting
on p. 18. (Parshall was the business partner of Elaine Thompson, an “attachment
therapist” who was involved in the case of the caged Gravelle children in Ohio;
Parshall was not accused in that case nor as far as I know was she involved in
the treatment of the Gravelle children.) Parshall’s article describes the use
of “neurofeedback” for treatment of severe behavior problems in an adopted boy.
Over a period of two years, the boy received the treatment (usually described
as “possibly efficacious”), and his behavior improved, an outcome that Parshall
attributes to neurofeedback rather than to maturation. Parshall states that
apparent regressions in behavior are to be expected at the beginning of the
treatment and that it is critical not to increase medications, but actually to
decrease them, at this point-- a potentially
dangerous statement if there are serious biological reasons for the problems,
and one which Parshall makes without explanation.
Interestingly, on p. 24, we see an article (and
accompanying advertisement) by Terry Levy and Michael Orlans, founding members of
ATTACh and formerly attachment therapists involved with Evergreen, CO (the home
of holding therapy), and still listing an Evergreen address. Levy was the
editor of a 2000 book published by Academic Press that featured a chapter by
Nancy Thomas, the “therapeutic foster parent”, who advised removing furniture
from children’s rooms and placing alarms on their doors, requiring them to ask
for all “privileges” including use of the toilet, limiting the quantity and
variety of their food, and insisting on periods of “strong sitting” without
motion or speech. Thomas also advocated withholding information from children
in treatment, including answers to their questions about when they would see
their parents again (these, of course, were children who were said not to be
attached to the parents they were asking for).
Speaking of Nancy Thomas, the piece by Julie Beem on
p. 30 seems to be on the same page with the Thomas philosophy. A sidebar lists “therapeutic
parenting” programs as suggested by Thomas, by Heather Forbes, by Katherine
Leslie (all fans of the restrictive approach), as well as by the Love & Logic
company, headed up by Foster Cline, one of the originators of holding therapy
and related approaches.
Have I said enough to show what’s going on here? I
could, but won’t, go on to discuss the numbers of therapists listed at the end
of the issue who have been associated with potentially harmful treatments for
children and with the less harmful but not demonstrably effective approaches now
sponsored by ATTACh. I do not see on that list the names of respected,
evidence-oriented clinicians like Mary Dozier or Sheila Eyberg!
Adoption
Today, can’t you do better than this? Adoptive parents need
information that is both reliable and accessible. A magazine that functions as
the house organ of ATTACh and as an advertising medium is not living up to its
responsibilities.
The "Adoption Today" April 2011 issue has more of the same. You can see a bit of the issue online:
ReplyDeletehttp://www.bluetoad.com/publication/index.php?i=65984&m=&l=&p=34&pre=&ver=flex
I would love to see what they list as "Attachment Resources" on page 32.