It’s so sad to see the number of queries that come
to this blog from terrified young mothers who have become addicted to Googling
about autism and want to know whether the (usually) typical infant behaviors
they see mean that their children will be autistic. Other than cursing these
insufficiently-informative “red flag” sites, there’s not much I can do except
to tell them that they will just have to wait to get a definitive answer,
because their babies are too young for anyone to tell whether there is an
autism diagnosis in their future.
I recently came across two articles in the very
helpful Brown University Child and Adolescent Behavior Letter (CABL) that
I would like to summarize-- not because
they tell how to diagnose autism early, but because they underline the problems
of trying to do so.
The first of these articles, by Stephen Sheinkopf,
is titled “Autism in infancy: Advances and implications for clinical practice”
(CABL, March 2013, pp. 1.4-5). Sheinkopf notes that “the presentation of autism
in infancy is subtle, with more obvious signs and symptoms emerging over the
first two to three years of life… these early developmental differences are
difficult to detect, making it a challenge to screen for the risk of autism in
early infancy. The gradual emergence of autism symptoms during development also
makes diagnosis challenging in later infancy and toddlerhood.” Even when
symptoms like poor communication development become evident, it may be far from
clear whether there are hearing difficulties, specific speech and language
disorders, or even mental retardation at work. Sheinkopf goes on to say that
there are “few reliable findings reported in infants younger than 12 months of
age”, even when children studied are known to be at risk for autism because
they have autistic siblings-- and as
Sheinkopf point out, it may not even be possible to generalize from those
at-risk children to the rest of the population.
Even when toddlers are diagnosed with autism, a
longitudinal follow-up is needed to be sure that diagnosis was correct.
Sheinkopf describes two composite cases (in other words, these are not specific
real children, but a narrative based on several similar cases). One case
involved a 30-month-old boy who had been born at less than 30 weeks’ gestation,
had been in intensive care for some time, and had normal motor development, no cerebral
palsy, and no hearing or vision impairment. He was quite delayed in language
and had some cognitive delays as well. He also showed behaviors that can be symptoms of autism—repetitive movements,
little eye contact, poor communicative abilities, and he received a tentative
diagnosis of autism. However, following early intervention services he improved
in language skills, performed fewer repetitive movements, and became more
social and playful with adults as well as becoming more interested in other
children than he had been. Although he continued to have some delays, by the
time he was 40 months old, the autism diagnosis was ruled out.
In Sheinkopf’s second case, a 20-month-old boy had
one older brother with autism, another with language delays, and a member of
the extended family for whom autism was a possibility. He did not speak at all
and did not vocalize much. He could solve problems that required no verbal
ability but did not respond to verbal instructions. He was affectionate and playful with his
parents and showed some interest in other children. He flapped his hands when
excited, but not in ways different from typical children of his age [I hope
readers will notice the fact that hand-flapping is common in toddlers]. It was not clear at
this point whether his diagnosis should be autism or language delay. Ten months
later, after early intervention services were received, it was seen that
although his language had improved it was repetitive and not necessarily
directed to other people. His social pragmatic skills had worsened compared to
where he was at 20 months. He had an increase in repetitive and stereotyped
actions and had begun to examine objects intensely in a way that interfered
with play or social activities. By his third birthday, the diagnosis of autism
given at 30 months continued to seem appropriate.
Notice that in both these cases the children were
seen to have factors that put them at risk for autism, but that even well past
the first birthday it was not clear whether their abilities and behaviors were
indicators of autism—it was simply seen that autism was one of the
possibilities. The first child became less “autistic-like” over time, but the
autism diagnosis was not ruled out until he was well over three years old. The
second child improved in language, but showed increasing evidence of autism and
was finally diagnosed at age 2 ½. The child diagnosed with autism became more
different from typically-developing children with age-- the differences were not clear-cut earlier in
his life.
In a second relevant piece in CABL, Alison Knopf (“Brief
observation not adequate to detect autism risk in young children”, March 2015,
pp. 3-4) described a study by Gabrielsen, Farley, Speer, et al., (in Pediatrics, 2025, Jan. 12). Knopf notes
that “autism spectrum disorder doesn’t manifest itself suddenly; symptoms
appear over time, with gaps between typical and atypical development widening
as the child gets older. Children have both typical and atypical behaviors [a
most important point—JM] but little is known about what the ratio of these
behaviors is during the 10-minute timespan of an average primary care visit.”
In the study by Gabrielsen et al., clinicians were asked to see whether they
could identify autism in children 15 to 33 months old, by watching two 10-minute
video clips of each child. Children who
were previously identified as autistic were missed in 39% of the cases,
suggesting that longer observations and more information than most pediatricians
get are needed for more accurate diagnosis.
In the Gabrielsen study, a very important point
concerned the fact that both typically-developing and autistic children had
some typical behaviors and some atypical behaviors. The autistic children
showed typical behaviors 89% of the time and atypical behaviors only 11%, while
the typically-developing children showed atypical behaviors only 2% of the
time. All the children were prompted to respond to their names at least once,
and 50% of the children in the typical group failed to respond once or more,
while 59% of children in an additional language-delay group did not respond on
one or more occasions. Of the children in the autism group, 58% responded to their name at least once, while 80% of the
typically-developing children responded at least once.
Making eye contact, responding to their name,
hand-flapping, repetitive behaviors—all of these are found in both autistic and
typically-developing toddlers. There is no single symptom or list of symptoms
that is a certain indication of autism even in children this old, and much less
can such a symptom be found for infants in the first year.
Although specialized early intervention services can
be helpful for toddlers and preschoolers, the best thing parents of infants can
do is to encourage good development by sensitive, responsive care, by plentiful
and playful social interaction that follows the child’s lead, and by minimizing
the screen use that so easily keeps parents from these first two development-supporting
activities. We would all like to think that a magic device could let us detect
autism early, and some other magic ritual could allow us to prevent atypical
development, but so far-- it just isn’t so,
and all parents can do is the best parenting they can.
What about this computer program that tracks eye movement and allowed to detect a very high correlation between a strongly marked preference for shapes rather than persons and autism?
ReplyDeleteFirst of all, there are going to be a lot of variations in this association. Autistic children act "typical" a large proportion of the time, and typical children act "autistic" quite often. Second, before such a test could be useful, it would be necessary to have tested large numbers of babies at different ages and established norms for visual preferences, which are going to be different at different ages. Keep in mind, too, that children who are diagnosed as autistic at an early age are quite often found to be typically-developing in another year or two-- many false positives, but I confess I know nothing about false negatives here.
Delete