Over the last several years, I have had dozens of
queries from concerned parents who had read some “red flag” websites that
describe symptoms of autism. These parents were terribly worried that their
babies did not make enough eye contact and that meant they would be autistic.
There have been a few cases where the parent’s description was worrisome, but
in most of the cases the problem was simply that the baby was very young. What those websites often
neglect to tell you is that young babies normally show many of the behaviors
that would be symptomatic of autism if
they were older. The autistic toddler or preschooler continues to show some
behaviors that are normal in earlier life; the typically-developing child stops
doing things that belong to earlier development. Autism is primarily
characterized by developmental delays, not by doing things that are never seen
in the lives of typically-developing children. For example, the hand-flapping
and toe-walking often noted as indications of autism are also seen in
typically-developing children, but at an earlier period of development.
In the last few weeks, in addition to the usual
questions about eye contact, I have noticed some new questions about motor
development and especially about the way a baby rolls over. I see references to
this issue on various chat groups and on Youtube too. They all refer back to a
particular publication, and I want to talk about that.
In the mid-1990s, the researcher Philip Teitelbaum
became interested in the idea that atypical movement patterns might precede and
predict more obvious symptoms of autism. This was by no means a silly idea—many
other neurological problems of childhood involve movement anomalies. It’s been
known for a long time that infants who already have “handedness” (they use one
hand consistently rather than alternating) or who crawl asymmetrically, with
one side of the body doing more work, most often have some damage to one side
of the brain. Cerebral palsy, which can interfere so badly with speech, may not
be diagnosed at birth, but it may be seen in anomalies of movement in the first
year. That autism might also involve problems of motor development was a reasonable
hypothesis.
In order to see whether babies who were later
diagnosed as autistic had unusual movement patterns later on, Teitelbaum
advertised and announced his work in various newspapers (this was before much Internet).
He asked for parents to send him home videos of their babies, so he could
examine movement patterns in early life and see whether he could identify any
unusual movements in those who were later considered autistic.
Teitelbaum and some co-authors published the results
of this study in 1998 in the Proceedings
of the National Academy of Sciences. They had found videos for 17 babies
who were later diagnosed as autistic, and reported that these babies showed
unusually movement patterns. Delays and asymmetries in sitting and walking were
evident, and rolling over was described as not only delayed and awkward, but as
involving a different use of head and legs to turn the body that is
characteristic of typically developing infants. However, there has been little
replication of this work, and there is ongoing discussion of the method that
should be used to observe and describe motor behavior (see http://asb.brain.riken.jp/files/Threelessonsfrom.pdf
).
There are several points to be made here. One is
that the rolling pattern described has not clearly been shown to be a precursor
of autism, and in fact Teitelbaum was criticized for the lack of control or
comparison information he provided. Failing to roll at all, past the usual time
when this milestone is achieved, is probably a more significant issue.
Second, and very important, Teitelbaum’s work
looked at the motor development of babies of whom the great majority slept in
the prone, tummy-down position. Babies in the United States, Australia, and
Great Britain today are very likely to be put to sleep supine, lying on their
backs. This practice is known to delay motor development compared to what was
seen years ago for prone-sleeping babies (see www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pubmed/15934485
). The sequence and timing of motor development Teitelbaum found in
typically-developing babies were not what we would see today in typically-developing
babies, so delays that Teitelbaum considered to be characteristic of
later-found-to-be-autistic infants cannot be used to assess babies today.
Third, every baby has his or her own schedule of
achieving motor milestones. There may be a long pause between two milestones,
or one may come very rapidly after another--
as when babies hardly creep at all, but go quickly to walking. There is
a normal range of development, not a deadline to get something accomplished.
That means that only a rather sophisticated observer will be able to detect
something out of sequence or dramatically delayed in motor development; Youtube
won’t do the job!
Fourth, let me point out that if a baby does have
unusual movement patterns, there can be many reasons for them other than
autism, and it is important to seek medical care if such patterns are seen.
Conclusion? The concern about how babies roll over has
recently been whipped up on various websites devoted to encouraging parents to
be anxious about the possibility of autism. Let me remind you that the present
high reported rates of autism are probably caused primarily by changes in diagnostic
approaches that include some minimal disturbances in the same category with
debilitating autistic disorders. Also, it is clear that quite a few children
who are diagnosed as autistic in the preschool period no longer have that
diagnosis several years later. Finally, because parents who are worried about
eye contact and rolling over also want to know about “early intervention”-- the best thing you can do for any baby is to
have fun together and respond to the baby’s communications as best as you can.
If you’re worried about movement problems, by sure you do plenty of tummy time.
Minimize screen time, and that means the time you are on your phone in the baby’s
presence as well as screens for the baby to look at. Talk to the baby a lot. All
babies, typical or atypical, will benefit from these actions.
No comments:
Post a Comment