When thinking about parental alienation (PA) issues in
the United States, I usually think it makes sense to consider a child age range
of about 9-17 years. It’s rare in the U.S. to have a child less than 9 who is claimed
to show PA when he or she avoids one parent. This narrow age range is one in
which issues about attachment—if any—are much different from those we see in
infants and toddlers. Some PA advocates, like Craig Childress, propose that
when a child avoids a parent this means that something has damaged her
attachment to that parent, or even “suppressed” her whole attachment system
[don’t ask me, I’m just reporting the news here], but this is hardly likely for
9-17-year-olds, whose social focus is less and less on their parents and more
and more on their peers.
However, it does seem that in some cases PA concepts
are applied to much younger children. My first introduction to PA occurred when
I was contacted by the grandmother of two preschoolers who had been taken from
the mother on the grounds that she had caused their avoidance of their father.
Those children were certainly at ages when attachment to a familiar caregiver
is the focus of a child’s life and when abrupt, long-term separation from that
person causes deep distress, grief, and lethargy that may last for months.
Some European colleagues are also telling me that they
see PA allegations in situations where toddlers and preschoolers are concerned.
They express concern over the impacts on young children of being forcibly
removed from a familiar parent and placed in the custody of one whom they
essentially do not know (their biological relationship being of little import
here). Others have argued, to the contrary, that one parent may prevent the
child from having contact with the other on the grounds that their attachment
will be harmed if that happens.
I want to comment on these highly fraught situations
where parents are in enormous conflict over the custody or parenting of a
preschool or younger child. First, though, I want to say that I am going to
talk only about the role of attachment in parenting decisions when all else is
equal. I mean: this is not about situations where there has been domestic
violence, or physical or psychological child abuse, or child sexual abuse. Those
all create their own unique dangers. I am going to talk only about situations
where one parent wants more contact with the child , the child seems reluctant,
and the other parent is resisting the change in contact-- but nothing else is wrong.
Point 1: How the child acts, and how parents can
handle this, depends largely on how old the child is.
A baby under 6
months of age is usually quite ready to socialize with whoever comes along and
has not yet formed an emotional attachment to any individual. The young baby
may object if handled clumsily by someone, or if someone fails to pick up on
cues about what the baby needs, but this is not about attachment or about fear.
However, even at 4 months or so, the baby may be quite distressed if someone
stares blankly at her and does not change expression or move in response to the
baby’s social signals.
After 6 months, but usually before 12 months, babies
begin to form emotional attachments to people whom they see frequently and who
behave toward them in sensitive and responsive ways. They often indicate that
this developmental change is occurring by showing fear of strange things and
even by being startled and frightened by some things that have happened before
(noisy garbage trucks, jumping dogs). They try to stay near familiar people and
avoid strangers—often, they will not look at a friendly stranger who approaches
them, and will even cry if the person keeps looking at them. If left in a
strange place without a familiar person, they cry and will not look around with
interest or try to explore, even though they will explore the same place if a
familiar person is there.
Babies of 10 or 12 months and toddlers also show a
behavior called “social referencing”. If they meet a new person or a new kind
of object, they look at the face of a nearby familiar person. If that person
looks frightened and seems to be looking at the new person or object , the baby
backs off and will not approach the unfamiliar thing. If the familiar person
smiles and looks relaxed, the baby goes ahead to explore the new person or
object.
As toddlers get to be two or three years old, and as
they learn to understand and use language, they can cope much better with
unfamiliar situations. They may go to child care or preschool, encounter new
babysitters, and develop better social skills. If they are tired or hungry or
sick, however, they still seek familiar people and reject everyone else. (Of
course, when sufficiently cross they may reject a familiar person too!) If they
cannot find the familiar person, or if their separation goes on for a long time
(weeks), they may show serious distress and take many weeks to recover; even if
reunited with the familiar person become clingy and anxious.
Some children are much more intense in their negative
reactions than others. They have temperamental differences that are
biologically determined. A child who takes a long time to adapt to child care
or who is predictably seriously frightened of clowns or masks or large groups
of people is also likely to show more intense reactions than others to separation
or to strange people and places.
Point 2: Can knowing about attachment behavior help
people?
Yes, knowing
these things helps parents know how to handle the distress of young children as
they visit a parent they do not live with—if the parents give it some thought.
The awareness that such distress is natural may (I hope) prevent suspicions or
allegations that one parent is causing the child to hate or fear the other.
Let’s look at possible things to do for different ages.
For the youngest babies, under 6 months, if the baby
is getting distressed about clumsiness or a withdrawn expression, the solution
is for the adult to learn to behave differently. More contact and more daily
care will help the adult learn to engage the baby in care routines to the
satisfaction of both. “Staring” or looking withdrawn is a different problem.
Some adults can do better if they know they are looking a certain way, but
others may be depressed, anxious, or distracted, and those things are harder to
handle. It will be helpful to everyone to know that the baby is responding to
the facial expression and not to the individual, however.
For older babies and toddlers, there are a lot of
issues that may need work by the adults. If one parent is actually frightened
of the other, a toddler doing social referencing may quickly catch on to this
and also be frightened, even though the frightened person has no wish for this
to happen. An older baby or toddler may also be frightened and resist a parent
who is not often present and may become distraught when approached—and even
more so if taken away from home. These are normal behaviors of typically
developing children and are not taught or caused by one of the parents. There
can be improvement if both parents behave calmly and give the child plenty of
time to warm up; there can also be worsening if people are hasty, argue, or are
demanding or rough. Toddlers are most comfortable in familiar places, so if it
is possible for the child to stay at home and the parents to switch places for
a time, the child will be more comfortable and will more quickly come to accept
both parents as familiar attachment figures.
Times of transition, including bed time, are always
the most difficult for toddlers and preschoolers to handle, even if there is no
family separation or conflict going on. In perfectly happy families, young
children may respond to a transition by pushing away one parent and saying they
want the other; the next day they may reverse who they want. It’s the
transition that’s the problem. So, when transferring a young child from one
parent to the other after divirce, it would be surprising if everything always
went smoothly. Crying when going from father to mother probably does not mean
that father abused the child. Crying when going from mother to father probably does
not mean that the mother has brainwashed the child to make her afraid of
father.
The very difficult problem here is that in a few cases
these things have happened, and it can be difficult to know what is distress
caused by bad events and what is normal transition complaining. But following
the “black swan” rule, our first assumption should be that things belong to the
more frequent category of events. If parents are prepared to deal calmly with
times of transition, knowing that they are likely to include child crying and
distress, everyone will be able to do better—to the advantage of the child, who
is not helped if adults feel and show their own upset. If this does not help, it's time to explore carefully to see whether there is something one or both parents are doing to distress the child.
It’s true that
one of the real difficulties of divorcing parents is the attachment-related
reaction of young children. They cry, they cling, they demand attention, they
avoid unfamiliar people even more than usual, they need a lot of emotional
support as they struggle to feel secure with both parents. This is all
occurring at a time when one or both of the parents may be completely stunned
by what is happening and want nothing more than to pull the covers over their
head and hide for a month. One of the natural problems of being a parent is
that the less you want demands placed on you, the more demands your young child
will place. You have “gone away” from your usual availability, and the child
wants you back.
It's tough on everybody. But let’s be careful about
interpreting normal child separation behaviors as “parental alienation” or
thinking that somehow the child’s internal working model of attachment has been
smashed by a visit or the lack of a visit. Jumping to those conclusions only
makes things worse in the long run.
No comments:
Post a Comment