Probably most interested people saw the movie Three Identical Strangers before I did—I
only got to it on an airplane recently. This semi-documentary story of triplets
born in the early ‘60s and adopted by separate families through a well-known
New York adoption agency is fascinating but does not really touch on all the
historical details.
As you may already know, the Identical Strangers, reared separately and with no knowledge of
each other, met by accident as young men, were fascinated by the immediate
relationship they formed, started a restaurant together, and became media
darlings to some extent. Looking into the circumstances of their separate
adoptions, they were able to find out that their placements had been part of a
study conducted by the child psychiatrist Peter Neubauer in an effort to examine the
effects of nature (heredity) and nurture (experiences of caregiving) on child
development. Each of the triplets recalled childhood events in which psychologists
or other observers came to their homes and tested their development in various
ways. The adoptive parents did not know that they had only one triplet per
family, and they accepted the testing as part of a study of normal child
development. In fact, the boys had been placed in homes that were expected to
present some contrast in childrearing methods—one affluent, one middle-class,
and one working-class, with the idea that these different homes would provide
different experiences for each of the three genetically identical children.
When the triplets and their adoptive parents found
that they had not been informed about the circumstances or the study, they were
deeply offended and angry, feeling they had been treated like lab rats or
guinea pigs. They began to search for
the still unpublished results of the study as well as attempting successfully
to find the identity of their biological mother. Peter Neubauer has died, and
the data collected in the study is sealed in the archives of the Yale Child Study
Center, not to be opened until 2060, when all or most of those involved in the
study will also be dead. The two surviving triplets are frustrated and
distressed by this and feel that it was wrong to keep them from knowing their
brothers. They have also found that a number of identical twin pairs were
placed apart and included in the research; some of them have reunited, but
there are probably some who still do not know they have a twin.
It’s very understandable that the triplets and some of
the twins have strong negative feelings about these events in their lives. The
fact that the adoptions took place through a Jewish agency, and many of the
children were Jewish, calls up horrible reminiscences of Nazi experimentation
on children. Today’s research ethics would not permit any such study to be done
now.
But is it reasonable to blame Peter Neubauer, the
cooperating adoption agency, or anyone else involved in the implementation of
the study for the choices they made? Today, yes, they would be considered culpable,
but if we look at historical background the conclusion is less clear.
First, let me point out that before reliable birth
control and legalized abortion, there were large numbers of “illegitimate” or
“unwanted” babies who needed care. Unmarried mothers were strongly discouraged
from keeping their babies. Even healthy, married mothers who had multiple
births might well be advised to give a baby for adoption because of practical
and medical issues that could arise from trying to rear two or three infants
together. (The famous Dionne quintuplets in Canada were actually taken to be
raised in a government institution because of this kind of reasoning.) The idea
of separating children at birth was very much a current one at that time, so it
is very likely that triplets would have been separated for adoption under any
circumstances; the Neubauer study was simply a matter of following up on the
children’s development after placement was already made. The placement of the
children in the three somewhat different homes was a little unusual, because
ordinarily adoption agencies tried to place children with adoptive families
whose socioeconomic status was about the same as the birth mother’s (birth
fathers did not come into the equation at this point). However, the three
families were all sufficiently well off to care for adopted children, and all
had already adopted at least one child successfully, so although one of the
boys may have had more luxuries than the others, no one suffered from poverty
or deprivation, and in fact the families were more similar to each other than
we would be likely to see in today’s increasing inequity.
How about the secrecy? Today, we encourage adoptive
families to learn all they can about the adopted child’s biological
background—even to enter into an open adoption where the birth mother stays in
contact with the child. We stress the need to be clear with the child about his
or her adoption from an early age. Adopted children today are much more likely
to be of different ethnicities from their adoptive parents and siblings, so
adoptive status is often easily visible to outsiders. These circumstances were
not usually the case when the triplets were born. Secrecy in adoption was seen
as essential for the birth mother, who could now go on with her life without
having to fear that anyone-- including a
future husband and children—would ever know about her distressing history.
Adoptive parents themselves often preferred to keep the adoption a secret out
of concern for the social stigma that known infertility might bring for them;
men especially might not want the adoption to be known, out of feared confusion
between infertility and impotence. If no one was to know about the adoption,
certainly the children could not be told, because they would be bound to ask
questions or tell people outside the family, and as a result many adopted
individuals found out that they were adopted only after the deaths of their
adoptive parents.
The Neubauer study also took place in the wake of
revelations about an earlier study that purported to investigate effects of
nature or of nurture in adopted children, especially in twins one of whom had
been adopted. A British psychologist, Cyril Burt, had conducted what appeared
to be extensive studies looking at the effects of heredity and environment on
intelligence. He published conclusions showing a strong effect of heredity on
intelligence, with twins reared in different families resembling each other
closely on intelligence tests. But Burt’s work turned out to have many flaws.
For example, one of a pair of twins might remain with the birth family while
the other one was placed with a cousin or aunt in the same village, and the
children might grow up going to school and playing together, so their
environments were really not different. In addition, it became plain after a
while that Burt had actually used fraudulent methods of analyzing and
presenting his data, publishing statistics that simply were not possible
calculations.
The Burt scandal meant that questions about heredity
and environment were completely unanswered. Peter Neubauer thought to do work
that would help to answer those questions with children who were already being
separated at birth and whose adoptive status was already being kept secret. The
one twist in placement of the triplets was the use of three families somewhat
different in socioeconomic status, but all perfectly capable of adopting and
rearing children. Neubauer’s work did not meet either the scientific or the
ethical standards of the present day, and the sealing and archiving of the
records does raise many questions, but the story of the triplets should not be
interpreted as evidence of cruelty or indifference to the needs of children and
of families. We can sympathize with the surprise and distress felt by the
triplets as the story unwound (and thank for their willingness to tell us their
experiences) without assuming that the study was conducted by “mad scientists”.
No comments:
Post a Comment