Proponents of “parental alienation” (PA) have
generally not been in a position to know whether, when a child of divorce
resists or refuses contact with one parent, the other parent has actually
worked to manipulate the child’s attitudes. The posited “alienating” behavior
would be hard to observe for anyone outside the household, and even parents who
have behaved in that way are likely to deny it if taxed with it.
PA proponents generally point to child behaviors as
evidence that the preferred parent must have influenced the child’s attitude
toward the other parent. Following Richard Gardner, they state that a list of
symptoms indicates that the child has been “alienated” from one parent.
Naturally this list is headed by resistance to or refusal of contact with the
non-preferred parent—although oddly enough, PA has been claimed in cases where
a child does not resist or refuse contact at all.
Gardner listed other symptoms of PA, based on his
clinical experience, and as far as I can see no systematic empirical work has
been done to show that these symptoms are more common in children who resist or
refuse contact than in any other children of similar age. Indeed, PA advocates
do not seem to have done any systematic testing of children to indicate that
the claimed symptoms are reliably found. This lack of evidence raises the
possibility that the child behaviors said to indicate alienation, where they
exist, are in fact simply characteristic of children and adolescents at certain
stages in their development, especially those wh
o are experiencing stress..
Let’s look at some of the claimed symptoms and see how
they compare to developmentally-expectable characteristics of cognition and
behavior.
1. 1. PA
proponents state that affected children not only resist or refuse contact with
a non-preferred parent, but give weak,
frivolous rationalizations about their refusal. For example, a child or
adolescent may cite the eating habits or other personal characteristics of a
parent as a reason to avoid contact. There are a number of explanations for these
apparently inadequate reasons for refusal. Some may have to do with other
people in the parent’s household or with poor parenting skills on the part of
the rejected parent, both currently and in the child’s past experience. In
addition, when personal characteristics or behaviors of the adult are referred
to, the problem may have to do with the appearance of misophonia, a sensory
processing and emotional disorder, in adolescence (see www.psychatrist.com/PCC/articles/Pages/2017/v19n05/17102106.aspx).
Both school-age children and young adolescents may have a great deal of trouble
identifying and communicating factors that lead to refusal of contact, as their
cognitive development makes it difficult for them to frame complex communications
in ways that are easily understood by others; this would be particularly true
when children are confronted by hostile adults bent on denigrating their
statements.
2. 2. PA
proponents state that alienation is shown when children and adolescents show a
lack of ambivalence or “black and
white thinking” about their preferred and non-preferred parents. But this
unsubtle view of life is characteristic of children whose thinking is at the
concrete operational stage (which may last in some ways until the late teens).
Concrete operational thinkers have many cognitive skills but do not think
abstractly or create hypotheses that can be tested against reality. Thinking
about multiple factors that operate simultaneously is most difficult for them,
as is seen when they are asked to solve problems about pendulums or levers;
they tend to focus on a single factor and ignore other possibilities. Thus,
concrete operational thinkers, of school age or in early adolescence, are not
likely to be able to consider multiple characteristics of parents and to
compare multiple causes of parental behavior, or to accept that a person can be
both bad and good at the same time. Even adults, whose tolerance for ambiguity
is much better than that of children, have a strong tendency to categorize
people as all good or all bad—in fact, we might say that concepts suggested by
PA proponents are a good example of this tendency to demonize one member of a
pair of people. Young adolescents, as they begin to develop the formal operational
thought that will allow them to consider several factors at the same time, tend
to thinking idealistically and are quick to reject any person whose bad
qualities seem to outweigh their good ones.
3. 3.. PA
proponents state that children who have been “alienated” from one parent display
the “independent thinker”
phenomenon—that is, they deny that anyone has influenced them and maintain that
they have come to their positions entirely on their own. However, it is characteristic
of school-age children to be unable to explain how they came to a belief,
because their concrete operational patterns of thought prevent them from
examining a range of factors that determined their ideas. As adolescents shift
their loyalties from parents to peers, they typically deny parental influences
on their ideas and are likely to assert their mental independence from any
adult (as in “not trusting anyone over thirty”). Children and young adolescents
generally have difficulty thinking about why they assume something is true, and
like adults may resent the idea that they have been manipulated or persuaded by
others.
4. 4. PA
proponents claim that children who resist or refuse contact with a parent are
marked as “alienated” because they do not
feel guilty about their treatment of that parent. This point is easily explained
by comments already made in this post; if a child or adolescent actually
believes that a parent is bad enough to reject, he or she will assume that it
is the parent who should be feeling guilty. In addition, although even quite
young children experience some kinds of empathy with others (and may try to get
adults to help a person who seems to be in trouble), human beings of any age
tend to empathize best with those who seem like themselves and are in need of
protection. To expect a child or adolescent to feel guilt toward or empathize
with a parent – to empathize with a person who neither resembles them nor needs
help—seems equivalent to parentalizing the child. When guilt and empathy are felt
toward a preferred parent, PA proponents speak critically of parentalization,
but they are equally concerned when a child does not empathize with or feel
guilty toward the non-preferred parent. Looking at this matter developmentally
requires us to go beyond cognitive characteristics of childhood and to look at
moral development, which has a less predictable sequence than cognition does.
Higher degrees of empathy are displayed when individuals are at the level Kohlberg
called “conventional morality”, with emphasis on rules associated with group
membership and shared relationships. Many people do not achieve this stage
until adulthood and even then tend to be limited in their capacity for empathy.
This examination of PA
claims and common characteristics of children and adolescents suggests that the
PA symptoms described may well indicate nothing more than a child’s age and
developmental stage.
No comments:
Post a Comment