The concept of attachment—a toddler’s wish to stay
near a familiar caregiver, especially when frightened, tired, sick, or injured,
or when in an unfamiliar place—has given rise to thousands of research studies
and much speculation sine it was formulated by John Bowlby in the 1950s. Some
of Bowlby’s work focused on the attachment experiences of children who later
turned out to be delinquents, so from an early point ideas about attachment have
been connected with explanations of undesirable behavior and predictions of
emotional problems.
The most established measure of attachment, the
Strange Situation Paradigm, uses an artificially slightly scary situation to
look at how toddlers respond to their mothers’ leaving them briefly in a
strange place, and the way they respond to reunion with her when she returns.
The great majority of young children behave in the Strange Situation in ways
that let researchers categorize them in one of three categories. The largest
number are classified as securely attached, and smaller proportions as either
insecure-avoidant or insecure-ambivalent in their attachment relationship to
the specific familiar adult who is with them in the test situation. All of
these categories are within a normal range of social and emotional development
, and although “secure attachment” sounds better than the other categories, it
is not necessarily strongly associated with any great developmental advantage.
These attachment classifications may change over time and may well be different
from other attachment classifications a child would receive if tested with a
different familiar person--- that is, the attachment classification is neither
permanent or “in the child”, but is changeable and “in the relationship”.
Because similar language is used, people may jump to the conclusion that insecure
attachments lead to so-called “insecure” adult behavior like lack of
self-confidence or jealousy or poor social skills, but this is not the case.
Insecure attachments are not considered the ideal for toddlers, but neither do
they require treatment to prevent current or later difficulties. In addition,
it is very clear that the Strange Situation Paradigm was developed for research
work comparing groups of children and not for clinical purposes—insecure attachment
classifications in individuals are not diagnoses.
Years after Bowlby’s work, the psychologist Mary Main
and her colleagues described a form of toddler behavior in the Strange
Situation that was different from the three primary classifications of
attachment. They referred to this behavior as “disorganized attachment”. Young
children who were classed as having disorganized attachment behaved in quite
unusual ways when reunited with their mothers after a very brief separation. Some
froze in place after starting to approach the mothers; some backed toward the
mothers; some simply collapsed to the floor. For their parts, the mothers, many
of whom had endured earlier traumatic experiences, often appeared frightened as
they looked at the children. It seemed
that the children needed and wanted contact with their mothers, but they had no
effective way to get this because of their own state of fearfulness, perhaps
associated with the mothers’ apparent fear. Not only was the relationship
between child and mother disorganized and inadequate to give the child needed
support and comfort, but children in this kind of relationship would not be
able to use their mothers as “secure bases” to give them confidence for
exploring and learning—one of the most important benefits of toddlers’
attachment.
Disorganized attachment has been thought of for years
as an important indication of the need for treatment of mother and child. But
recent work suggests that it is not completely clear how disorganized
attachment develops, so it is in turn not completely clear what to do about it.
In one recent article (Duschensky, R. [2018]. Disorganization, fear, and
attachment: Working towards clarification. Infant
Mental Health Journal, 39, 17-29) , the author suggests three different
pathways by which toddlers may have arrived at disorganized attachment
behavior: serious rejection by the mother, traumatic experiences leading to
emotional dysregulation, and temperamental characteristics present at birth.
These different possibilities would suggest different treatment approaches, so
simply screening young children for disorganized attachment does not
necessarily give useful guidance about what help to offer. And, once again, the
Strange Situation Paradigm was never intended to make clinical decisions about
individuals, but was created as a way to compare groups for research purposes.
Research on disorganized attachment has looked at
whether this form of toddler behavior toward a mother is associated with
abusive treatment. It’s easy to see why this question would be asked, because
two of the pathways to disorganized attachment mentioned above – rejection by
the mother and traumatic experiences—could be connected with abuse. Indeed,
there are weak statistical relationships between child abuse and disorganized
attachment. But these correlations unfortunately have led some practitioners to
the idea that disorganized attachment behavior can be used to screen families
for child abuse. This conclusion is wrong for various reasons. One is the
oft-repeated but equally oft-forgotten fact that correlation of two events does
not show that one causes the other. It’s possible that abusive treatment of a
child could cause that child to show disorganized attachment behavior, but it’s
also possible that children who for other reasons show unusual attachment
behavior could trigger both fear and abusive treatment in their caregivers.
Even more likely, additional factors like poverty and family trauma could cause
both disorganized attachment behavior and abusive treatment, or could cause one
or the other separately. In any case, disorganized attachment behavior cannot
be used as a proxy measure or screen for child abuse; however much time and
resources this approach might save, it would not find all cases of child abuse,
and it would find many false positives as children who had never been treated
abusively could still show disorganized attachment behavior.
A recent article on disorganized attachment, written
by a large number of well-known attachment experts, has clearly stated the
limits of usefulness of the disorganized attachment classification ( Granqvist,
P., et al. [2017]. Disorganized attachment in infancy: A review of the
phenomenon and its implications for clinicians and policy-makers. Attachment & Human Development, 19,
534-558 ). The paper commented on four assumptions about disorganized
attachment that they characterized as false and misleading. These were first,
that attachment measures can be used to evaluate individual children in
judicial or child protective contexts; second, that the presence of disorganized
attachment behavior reliably shows that a child has been maltreated; third,
that disorganized attachment behavior reliably predicts emotional or behavioral
problems in the future; and fourth, that disorganized behavior indicates a
lasting characteristic of the child rather than one that can be influenced by
changed circumstances. The last assumption, particularly, is contradicted by
the fact that it is not unusual to see some mild level of disorganized attachment
behavior occur in toddlers in the Strange Situation, especially if they come
from challenging environments.
If children show organized insecure responses in the
Strange Situation, and if their families need treatment or services, a goal may
be to increase attachment security, but this does not mean that insecurity is
or predicts serious problems. If toddlers show severely disorganized attachment
behavior, they and their families may well need help for one or more reasons,
and various kinds of help (as well as maturation) may encourage better
organization of relationship-related behavior. However, it should never be
assumed either that clinical work with individuals can usefully be based solely
on the observation of disorganized
attachment, or that disorganized attachment shows that a child has been
maltreated and signals the need for authorities to intervene in the family.
Removal of the child should not be contemplated simply because of disorganized
attachment behavior.
No comments:
Post a Comment