I recently received an email from an anti-abortion
group who referenced a recent study about the visual behavior of 34-week fetuses—still
6 weeks before they would typically be born. Although the number of abortions
that take place at that point in gestation is vanishingly small, my
correspondents wanted to use the study to argue against terminating pregnancy
at any point. That’s what they do, so I won’t argue with it, but I’d like to
look at the study itself and what it means about early development of human
beings.
You can find a discussion of the study in a number of
places, but here’s one you might try: www.usatoday.com/story/news/2017/06/08/womb-view-fetuses-can-recognize-faces-while-still-inside-mom/102625620/.
The researcher was able to shine a bright light through the uterus and amniotic
fluid that surrounds the fetus and at the same time to do imaging that would
reveal how the fetus turned toward the light. What does this have to do with
faces? Well, there were two kinds of lights, each involving three dots arranged
in a triangle, which adults might perceive as “two eyes” and “one mouth”. The
lights were shown either in a position where they looked like the basic
elements of a face, or “upside down” so that the “mouth” was above the “eyes’.
The babies moved to “look at” the right-side-up face more than at the upside
down non-face.
Because the fetuses discriminated between the two
patterns by moving differently, it was concluded that they could were reacting differently
to things like faces than to other things that were not so face-like. It was
that discrimination that led to the statement that they recognize faces while
in the womb. It would be less exciting but a good deal more accurate to say
that they respond differently to the face-like triangle than to the other. They
do not recognize faces in the adult sense of being able not only to tell one
actual face from another but even to know which person has which face. No such
claims were made, and for good reason. You might as well say that unborn babies
recognize point-down triangles as that they recognize faces in the sense that
even a three-month-old does.
Under ordinary conditions, there is no light inside
the uterus, so an unborn baby has no visual experience whatsoever. He or she
has had no opportunity to learn what people, dogs, moons, or crib bumpers look
like. Any visual response that occurs—either in the womb or right after a
preterm or full-term birth—has to be produced by functions that are built into
the visual system as a result of genetic commands to the developing brain.
(Brain? Yes, because developmentally the retina or light-sensitive part of the
eye is actually a part of the brain.)
The visual system is the last sensory system to begin
its development in the prenatal period, so we might not expect vision to be
very good at 34 weeks gestational age—it’s not great at 40 weeks or full term.
When babies look at faces after birth, they tend to scan the eye areas and the
mouth area a lot, but as is shown by all the comments I get from mothers
worried about lack of eye contact, the very young babies do not look at faces
very much. They just look at them more than they look at other things. Babies
in the first month or so after birth are also very limited in their ability to
see objects that are too close or too distant, so even then they do not get a
lot of time to look at and learn about faces.
So how does it come about that before birth they can “look”
at a face-like pattern and respond to it as if it means something special to them?
The most likely reason is that in their still very immature brains they already
have feature detectors that are activated by a seen pattern. A feature detector
is a cell in the visual system of the brain that is connected to a particular
part of the retina and responds only to certain kinds of images that fall on
that retinal area. This is a lot easier to demonstrate with frogs than with
human beings (who tend to excuse themselves when you want to do things to their
brains), but frogs have feature detector cells that are “fly detectors” and
become activated when an image the size and shape and speed of a nearby moving
fly falls on the retina. The frog feature detectors signal the tongue, and zip,
the tongue nails the fly. Mammals too have feature detectors, although
fortunately they do not make us catch flies with our tongues. Face detectors
are among them—brain cells that are activated when the image of a shape that
resembles a face falls on part of the retina. Even sheep have face detectors
that respond to human faces. Sheep “recognize” faces in the same sense that
unborn babies do.
It’s really interesting and important for researchers
to show how early and how gradually human sensory and other systems begin to
develop. Those systems don’t just “come on line” instantaneously at some point,
and although birth is a dramatic event in the course of development, not all
developmental changes are closely related to it. However, when the researchers have done their
job, it’s also important for the media and their audience not to jump to
conclusions about the implications of findings. We humans don’t seem to have
feature detectors that respond to exaggerations!
No comments:
Post a Comment