Of the comments and queries I receive on this blog,
the greatest number are from parents concerned that their babies are showing
signs of autism. Of questions about autism, almost all of them focus on a lack
of eye contact—the babies do not gaze at their parents’ faces as much as the
parents expect them to.
Most parents, and certainly many Internet authors who
discuss autism, assume that whatever are the signs and symptoms of autism in
older children and adults, those will also be the signs and symptoms of autism
in young infants. They know that social awkwardness and a lack of eye contact
and other communicative gestures are common among older children with autism
spectrum disorder (ASD), so they assume that infants who are fated for an ASD
diagnosis will also lack eye contact. But in making this assumption they miss
two important points.
The first point is that infants in the first two or
three months are not easily attracted to pay attention to people. They will do
it now and then, but often they respond only to quite dramatic adult facial
expressions with wide open-mouthed smiles. Years ago, this developmental period
was referred to as a stage of normal autism—the word “autism” deriving from the
Greek word for “self”, and the babies being focused on themselves rather than
the environment. Now that people are terrified about ASD, one doesn’t come
across this expression, normal autism, any more, but that doesn’t mean that the
stage no longer exists. What would be a symptom of ASD in an older child is a
sign of perfectly normal development in a young infant. There is no point in
expecting a baby of a few months to make extensive eye contact, any more than
there would be any point in expecting her to build a tower of two blocks or to
spoon-feed herself.
My second point is that earlier and later behavior
patterns may be remarkably different in cases where there are developmental
problems. A good example is the pattern shown in Williams syndrome, a genetic
syndrome resulting from loss of certain parts of a chromosome. Williams
syndrome is not terribly debilitating, but it does cause developmental changes
that are rather different from typical development. Young babies with Williams
syndrome are terribly colicky and cry frantically no matter what is done to
soothe them. They are not interested in other people at that point. But when
the colicky stage passes, they become extremely interested in people, stare at
them intently, and appear to be “starved” for eye contact. We might expect them
at this point to be very interested in communication and to speak early, but
no; in fact, their speech is delayed by about a year. Once they do start to
speak, they became chatty conversationalists. As adults, they are still talkative,
with wonderful language abilities, and highly sociable—but socially awkward at
the same time because they seem to lack the social anxiety that guides most of
us. The screaming colic and delayed speech of the younger Williams syndrome
individual are by no means symptoms of either the strengths or the weakness of
the Williams adult—and it’s very possible that a similar situation holds for
ASD people, whose later symptoms may not mirror the earlier ones (if there are
any).
A possible conclusion from these two points is that
the current preoccupation of parents with eye contact may be irrelevant to the
diagnosis of autism. The fact that older ASD children may not use eye contact
for communication very much does not mean that we can identify infants who will
later be diagnosed with ASD by looking for them to make eye contact.
A recent paper on autism provides some interesting insights into
possible early symptoms of autism. (Thomas, M., Davis, R., Karmiloff-Smith, A.,
Knowland, V., & Charman, T. (2016). The over-pruning hypothesis of autism. Developmental Science, 19, 284-305.)
This is a very complicated paper, and I am only going to refer to one of its
points here.
The Thomas paper is one that discusses an idea about
how ASD develops. The basic idea is that autism results when a particular
problem occurs during early development. It is well known that during the first
year, there is great overdevelopment of synapses or connections between neurons
in the brain, followed by disappearance of many that are little used—a process
sometimes called “pruning”. Although some authors have suggested that autism
results from too little “pruning” of synapses, Thomas and his co-authors
hypothesize that too much “pruning” could be the problem. They have tested this
hypothesis by developing a neurocomputational model to allow them to predict
what kinds of problems should result from excessive “pruning”.
Like all good scientists, Thomas and his colleagues
are testing their model against some longitudinal studies of development of
autistic children, and to some extent are finding that the longitudinal studies
show the symptoms they predict on the basis of the timing of pruning events
during early development. These do NOT include symptoms of social interaction
problems. They do include difficulties with sensory development like over- or
under-sensitivity to sound or touch stimulation, and difficulties with motor
development. Motor development problems as a precursor to autism have been
discussed since the 1980s, when studies of home videos were sought by
researchers as a way to see the early development of children later diagnosed
with ASD. Even before that, clinicians had noted that unusual movement patterns
like crawling asymmetrically or always reaching with one hand during early
childhood were related to a variety of later developmental problems. These
sensory and motor problems in the first year or two may indicate that children
will later show the social interaction problems often associated with autism—even
though the children when younger do not show unusual social interactions.
The sensory and motor forerunners of autism are not
yet clearly understood, so they cannot be used for accurate identification of “pre-autistic” babies. In
addition, many young children who are thought to be autistic at age 2 show
normal development later. Much as we might like to have early identification
and early intervention, we don’t have it yet. But if identification and
intervention are ever going to work, they will have to be focused on
development that is really not typical—and it is quite typical for babies in
the first weeks and even months to look at things other than faces a good deal
of the time.
No comments:
Post a Comment