For a very
long time, before attachment became a religion, Americans have been worried
about “insecurity”. People who value independence and individualism can get
nervous about insecurity, with its suggestions of emotional dependence and social
engagement. I remember overhearing a conversation years ago, about a child who
had a “security blanket”. “How come he carries that blanket around all the
time?” “I guess he’s insecure.” “Oh. Well, I guess then we’d better take it
away from him.” (Here we have not only the belief that everyone must at least
behave as if they are independent, but a common error of logic-- if A causes B, then removing B must also
remove A!) Like many other somewhat frightening things, insecurity has been a
topic of humor from Linus right on to Woody Allen.
Today, we see the concern with insecurity reflected
in journalism and discussions that equate insecure attachment, as measured by methods
like the Strange Situation, with “attachment disorders”. Insecure attachment,
originally defined as a pattern or quality of attachment, may now be touted as
an emotional disorder (e.g., www.helpguide.org/mental/patenting_bonding_reactive_attachment_disorder.htm).
According to a number of studies, among children
with normal family experiences, about 65% are evaluated as securely attached.
About 30% are insecurely attached in one or another of the possible categories,
and a small percentage are assessed as disorganized in their attachment
behavior. Disorganized attachment is associated with early maltreatment and
also with later externalizing problems (e.g., aggressive behavior). Insecure attachment
is not ideal, but on the other hand is not particularly associated with serious
psychopathology.
If insecure attachment does not cause serious
problems, how did it come about that people are so concerned with it-- indeed, that they are concerned about it at
all? How did “security” come into the study of attachment? There is a history
here.
It’s easy to assume that secure and insecure
attachment behaviors are so different that they jumped right out at Mary Salter
Ainsworth as she observed young children. Actually, though, Ainsworth went
looking for them and developed the Strange Situation as an instrument that
would differentiate them. She has told part of the story herself
(www.psychology.sunysb.edu/attachment/online/ainsworth_security_and_attachment.pdf).
Ainsworth was a student of William Blatz of the University of Toronto in the 1930s. Blatz developed a “security theory” that was a welcome alternative to Freudian theory in an anti-psychoanalytic intellectual environment. Blatz did not publish widely or sponsor a great deal of research on security theory, and Ainsworth’s recollections and comments are probably the most accessible source of his ideas. As Ainsworth noted about Blatz, “He was a brilliant hypothesizer, but I believe he did not think of research so much as a way of testing hypotheses as a way of reformulating old hypotheses and discovering new ones. He did not attempt to spin a theory to encompass all of personality and its development. He did, however, think that the concept of security could guide the exploration of this rich and confusing field.”
Ainsworth was a student of William Blatz of the University of Toronto in the 1930s. Blatz developed a “security theory” that was a welcome alternative to Freudian theory in an anti-psychoanalytic intellectual environment. Blatz did not publish widely or sponsor a great deal of research on security theory, and Ainsworth’s recollections and comments are probably the most accessible source of his ideas. As Ainsworth noted about Blatz, “He was a brilliant hypothesizer, but I believe he did not think of research so much as a way of testing hypotheses as a way of reformulating old hypotheses and discovering new ones. He did not attempt to spin a theory to encompass all of personality and its development. He did, however, think that the concept of security could guide the exploration of this rich and confusing field.”
What was security, from Blatz’s viewpoint? According
to Ainsworth, it was “feeling confident or effective, even though one’s feeling
of efficacy might stem from reliance on something or someone other than oneself.”
Blatz felt that people want to be comfortable and
secure, but they were also motivated by an “appetite for change”, which in
itself can create insecurity. When very young, they experienced an “immature
dependent security”, in which they needed to be able to retreat to a protective
parent when frightened by the consequences of their exploration. Subsequently,
as children became more knowledgeable, they could achieve an “independent security”
that allowed them to explore without constantly checking back with adults. In
fact, Ainsworth noted, Blatz thought that by the time they reached maturity, “they
should be fully emancipated from parents and not dependent on them any more…
any substantial continuation of ‘immature dependent security’ was to be viewed
as undesirable”-- a far cry from the
current preoccupation with maintaining early attachment behavior through the
teens! Having achieved independence, however, the individual was now ready to
enter into a partnership of “mature independent security”, in which each
partner provided support and protection for the other as needed.
If steps in the development of security did not
progress properly, Blatz considered, a variety of processes like intolerance of
disagreement and blaming of others might come into the picture as ways to achieve
a sense of comfort and effectiveness. However, as Ainsworth pointed out, “Blatz
arrived at his list [of processes] in a purely ad hoc fashion in the course of his clinical experience, and never
claimed it was complete.”
Ainsworth’s early research involved attempts to
evaluate the security of adults. In pursuing this work, and in her later
efforts to assess children’s attachment, she followed an important concept
contributed by Blatz. As she wrote, “Blatz’s theory does not hold with a
simplistic dichotomy between secure and insecure. The degree to which children
are secure can be assessed through an examination of their confidence in
others, especially parents, to provide comfort, reassurance, and protection
when needed, and [my italics—JM]
their confidence in being able to cope with the world on the basis of their own
skills and knowledge… One cannot arrive at a single security/insecurity score,
but should instead consider the patterning
in a comprehensive assessment.” Such patterns, of course, are what
Ainsworth and her colleagues attempted to assess by means of the Strange
Situation, and that much-valued measurement seems unfortunately to have
encouraged not only a dichotomy of secure vs. insecure, but an overvaluing of
the former and a pathologizing of the latter. (Not all students of attachment
agree with the typology of secure and insecure attachment, and some authors
have proposed that attachment security actually lies on a continuum.)
As you can see, Blatz’s view of security seems to
have been a major factor causing Ainsworth to look for “secure” and insecure”
patterns in young children’s attachment behavior. She did not first look for
patterns, and then label them as secure or insecure. (Disorganized/disoriented
attachment behavior, on the other hand, was first noticed as a pattern and
later named.) Ainsworth continued, very appropriately, to focus on patterns of
attachment behavior, but unfortunately popular and journalistic views have come
to identify secure attachment as a good category and all other types of
attachment behavior-- insecure patterns,
disorganized/disoriented attachment, even Reactive Attachment Disorder-- as belonging to an equally bad category. This
is comparable to assuming that there is only one acceptable pattern of early
language development or of pubescence, and that all others -- not just the truly problematic---are to be
deplored or subjected to treatment.
It is a curious pursuit, the examination of the
journalistic theory of attachment. In some cases, like the one just discussed,
the media have re-worked earlier views in order to achieve a simple way of
defining the good and the bad, and in order to abandon the complicating idea of
developmental change in attachment. In
other cases, some of Bowlby’s old tenets, now long abandoned, are maintained.
For example, the statement that “you don’t attach in a group home” (http://miami.cbslocal.com/2014/04/10/senate-group-home-proposal-spurs/debate/)
may well be a result of assuming that Bowlby’s original monotropy theory
(attachment to a single caregiver only) still holds, although in fact this idea
was abandoned long ago, and it is clear that a young child may be attached to
several adults, not all necessarily with equal security.
No comments:
Post a Comment