Attachment theory,
as formulated decades ago by John Bowlby, is a framework for understanding how
human social interactions and relationships develop from infancy onward. Any
theory works to pull together observations or other data on a topic and to
suggest how they are connected with each other. Attachment theory deals with
observable aspects of social relationships such as the apparent indifference of
infants in the first months to contact with strangers, the quickly-developing
preference for familiar people and fear of separation or strangers as infants reach the end of the first year, the use of
contacts with familiar people to help toddlers explore and learn, and the
associations between early social experiences and adult attitudes toward other
people.
Attachment theory, as it is conventionally
understood, has been tested and revised as thousands of empirical studies have
examined it. This is not the case, however, for an “alternative” view of
emotional development that also uses the term attachment, but is in fact a
matter of attachment myths.
The organization “Focus on the Family” appears to
have bought into prevalent attachment myths. Although materials on its web site
reference John Bowlby and attachment theory, it is in fact the myths that are
repeated. Advice to parents given by “Focus
on the Family” is based on attachment myths, not on attachment theory.
Here is one example, taken from http://www.focusonthefamily.com/parenting/adoptive_families/attachment_and_bonding/new_definition_of_attachment-regulation.aspx.
. The FoF author, Debi Grebenik, says this: “Children (biological or adopted)
who do not get their needs met as babies and small children typically do not
form a strong attachment with their parents. Even when adopting a baby, it is
important to consider that the removal of a child from his or her biological
mother creates a traumatic event in the life of the child.” Grebenik provides a
diagram of the so-called “attachment cycle” that is solely a part of the
attachment myths system and in no way a part of attachment theory.
Grebenik’s statements vary between the deceptive and
the false. Certainly, young children are helpless to feed and care for themselves,
and for good development must have caregivers who will do these jobs and do
them well. However, it is a mistake to conflate good caregiving with the social
interactions that are the actual cause of emotional attachment to caregivers. This
aspect of attachment mythology adopts Sigmund Freud’s belief that children become
attached to familiar people because those people provide food, an idea that was
contradicted by Bowlby’s report, and the position of attachment theory, that
pleasant social interactions with an adult are the actual cause of a child’s
attachment to that adult.
Ordinarily, of course, caregivers who are neglectful
or abusive are also likely to fail in providing pleasurable social
interactions, and those who are attentive, sensitive, and responsive are also
likely to create pleasant social interactions with their babies. It is easy to
confuse these issues and to jump from care experiences to the social events
that create attachment. If Grebenik is going to write about these issues,
however, she should realize that it is deceptive to focus on satisfaction of
needs as a cause of attachment, when in fact social interactions are the
important factor here. Although events depicted in the “attachment cycle”
diagram are usually accompanied by the
important social interactions (which go unmentioned), the two are not the same
things. This may seem like nitpicking--
except for the fact that attachment mythologists often recommend
treatment of older children by attempts to re-enact the notional attachment
cycle, such as spoon- or bottle-feeding. Buying into attachment myths in this
way encourages parents and practitioners to choose forms of treatment that are
neither plausible nor demonstrably effective.
In referring to the removal of a child from the
biological mother as traumatic, Grebenik is again deceptive in her omission of
important details. Of course, separation from a familiar caregiver (biological
relative or not) is likely to be traumatic when two circumstances are present. The
first is that the child has already formed an attachment to a familiar person,
an event that does not occur before 6 months of age at the earliest. The second
is that the new caregiver is unavailable, insensitive, and unresponsive. (I
should note, by the way, that Bowlby did not consider, the second factor to be
important, but his colleague John Robertson demonstrated that it was.) If the
child is under 6 months old and is moved to a situation where a small number of
attentive adults give good care and are socially responsive, this does not appear
to be traumatic. Even an older child who is given sensitive, comforting care
will adjust well over time. However, a child old enough to have formed an
attachment, but too young to have developed good cognitive and language skills,
is likely to be traumatized if placed in the care of a busy, unavailable,
insensitive, and unresponsive caregiver. Children between 6 months and two
years of age are most likely to have the reaction that Grebenik appears to
ascribe to all children.
Let’s look at another FoF statement, at http://www.focusonthefamily.com/parenting/adoptive_families/wait-no-more/attachment-problems-up-close-and-personal.aspx.
Here, in a piece by Kelly and John Rosati, we see the advice that emerges
(logically but incorrectly) from the attachment myths described by Grebenik.
Here’s what they say about the advice given to adoptive parents of a baby,
whose age is not stated, but who was at the crawling stage. The adviser (an
adoptive parent, not a professional ) inquired whether the baby held his own
bottle, and whether when crawling toward an interesting object he looked back “to
show it to you”. When the parents replied Yes and No, respectively to these
questions, their adviser looked grave and announced that they were “in for
trouble” if they did not work to overcome these attachment deficits.
[Let me take a moment here to point out two things. The
first is that because there is no evidence that the “attachment cycle” described
earlier actually exists, there is no reason to think that holding or not
holding a bottle has any relevance to attachment. As for the second, the
adviser appears to be confusing two developmental steps. At about 10-12 months,
well-developed babies display “joint attention” by looking back and forth from
an interesting object to a caregiver, until they get the caregiver to look at
the object “with them”. This is not
usually considered a measure of attachment. The step this seems to have been
confused with is secure base behavior, in which a child exploring a strange
place or situation will occasionally make contact with a familiar person, by
coming back to the adult, by “checking back” with a look, or by calling to the
adult. Unless an interesting object was
also frightening, no one would expect a baby in a familiar setting to do much secure
base behavior as he or she explored. ]
What did the adviser then suggest? “…only John and I
should hold Daniel, and only I should feed him (not even John!). She told us
that we should never let him hold his own bottle; he needed to depend on me to
provide him with what he needed. … And then came the kicker: I needed to hold
him and be face-to-face with him for almost eight hours a day!”
This mother recognized the implausibility of the
advice-- although she apparently did not
think of the interference with Daniel’s normal mastery motivation and with the
eight hours of normal activity he would be missing, or of the training in
passivity that was being given here. Scared of the dire predictions of the
adviser, she did as she was told. “After several months of this therapeutic
parenting…Daniel began to make good eye contact… and was more engaged and more
emotionally connected with us”. The mother attributed the change she perceived
to the treatment given-- although in
fact one might well expect several more months in an adoptive family to be
accompanied by increasing maturity and attachment to the new caregivers.
(Indeed, I would ask whether those changes were actually slowed by the “therapeutic
parenting”!)
There you have it. “Focus on the Family”, an outfit
that draws many readers because of its religious and social positions, has bought
into a mythology of attachment that contradicts conventional attachment theory and that culminates
in diagnoses and treatment that are inaccurate and inappropriate. How about it,
FoF? Isn’t it time to replace the attachment myths with evidence-based material
about attachment--- and help rather than
hinder parents?
Have you ever adopted a child who was severely neglected in the first several years of life? Have you ever cared for a child--not so called treated, actually cared for--with attachment issues? Have you ever experienced what numerous adoptive and foster parents deal with on a daily basis? I know that a handful of attachment practices are crocks but those bad eggs give the rest a bad wrap. A myriad of attachment issues that go untreated so when something works, a parent desperate to make these child's life better and give them better coping skills you go with it. What exactly was your "scientific" approach to determining that the attachment created between the baby and mother you reference was not because of the therapeutic parenting? I think maybe you were not held enough as an infant!
ReplyDeleteI'm publishing this as a wonderful example of the ad hominem arguments so typical of supporters of attachment therapy.
Delete