An interesting story came up the other day at a
discussion on childrearing sponsored by the Delaware Valley group of the World
Association for Infant Mental Health. One participant told a story that’s quite
relevant to some of the recent discussion on this blog of punishment as a
parenting technique, modeling of aggressive behavior, and so on.
Her story was this: a young mother played roughly
with her year-old son, slapping him lightly but repeatedly until he became
distressed and frustrated. The mother was amused by what she was doing and by
the child’s frustration and attempts to fight back. If he slapped back at her
too much, though, she became infuriated and beat him. When the child got a
little older, he went to live with a relative, and there he showed that he had
learned that exchanging slaps was “play” and the right way to interact with
older people. The relative tried to teach him not to slap, but to use some
other form of physical contact like tickling or kissing, and this worked to
some extent, but the toddler would still occasionally approach an adult with a
slap. Within the family, people understood the background, but his behavior was
not welcome in out-of-home child care and early intervention settings.
Because of the physical force involved, this sounds
like an extreme case, but it’s certainly common for adults or older kids to
tease young children unmercifully, to justify their actions by saying they’re
“just teasing”, and to become angry and even punish the child who displays
distress when teased intensely. School-age children exposed to this treatment
usually learn how to evade the adult’s attention, but younger ones naturally
respond to distress by approaching the people who ought to be protecting them-- the very ones who are tormenting them. We’ve all seen this at the beach, as
screaming toddlers and preschoolers are carried to be thrown into the water, or
in other situations where an adult picks up a child and won’t put her down, or
threatens to throw away a toy or harm a pet. These situations often conclude
with some type of punishment of the child, who is then characterized as a
“wimp” or a “sissy”.
Infants too are teased, although their language and
cognitive development is not good enough for them to understand most threats or
insulting words. Even actions that an older person might think of as
potentially threatening-- like an adult
pretending to drink up a baby’s bottle--
may just seem funny to the baby, who doesn’t envision a world without
milk. Some play sequences seem to verge on teasing while never really getting
there-- for example, holding out a toy
for a baby to grasp, pulling it away, holding it out again, and repeating this
several times before letting the baby take the toy. When this stops before the
baby gets distressed, it’s certainly play; when it’s pressed until the baby
reacts with fretting or tears, it’s turned into teasing.
Most adults who play with or mildly tease babies
will stop when the game goes too far, or when the child is old enough to be
disturbed and confused by the adult’s ambiguous behavior. But others, like the
mother mentioned earlier, will escalate their teasing as the child gets to the
toddler stage. Little boys, especially, may be encouraged to get angry and
“fight” with other people, then be punished if their fighting gets too intense,
or ignored if the adult is suddenly bored with the whole thing.
Why is being teased so disturbing for toddlers and
preschoolers? The basic problem is that the adult’s communication is so
ambiguous. If the person is friendly, why is he threatening or frightening the
child? If he is hostile, why is he smiling and laughing? All of us find these
mixed messages confusing and concerning, but young children have developed
little skill in understanding how apparently contradictory emotions and facts can
exist side by side. They have not developed much ability at what the
psychologist Simon Baron-Cohen (yep, Sasha’s brother) and others call Theory of
Mind--- the ability to make accurate guesses about other people’s knowledge,
beliefs, and intentions.
Children under the age of three can already figure
out certain things about what others know or want, but they are not clear on
whether people can deliberately “trick” people or tease them by feigning a
belief or intention. In a study by Vanderbilt, Liu, and Heyman (The development
of distrust.[2011]. Child Development, 82,1372-1380),
children watched an actor who pretended he did not know where something was and
misled another person who was looking for the object. Three-year-olds trusted
information from that person as much as information from an actor who helped
another person. Four- and 5-year-olds trusted the “tricker” a bit less than the
“helper”, but only 5-year-olds said the helping person was “nicer” than the
other. This suggests that the toddler I mentioned at the beginning of this post
would not have been able to interpret some of his mother’s slapping as teasing
play, and other slaps as seriously meant. For him, slapping became just another
mode of interacting with adults, even when they appeared not to like it (after
all, his mother seemed not to like it sometimes, but then she would start it up
the next time).
Why did the mother behave this way? Not
surprisingly, the discussion of this point began with a suggestion that has
appeared often in recent comments on this blog: that she was repeating what had
been done to her when she was a child. This is certainly a possibility, and I
would not want to reject it without evidence. However, I’d like to point out that this is not the only
possibility, nor is it likely that such a complex behavior would have only one
cause. I would suggest also that the mother saw friends and relations “play”
with their children and even with each other by pretending to fight. In
addition, her slapping play may have been one of the few ways of playful interaction
with a child that she was aware of. All of us initiate play with children
because we enjoy the way they respond to us--
for our own fun--- and we often carry out that play in stereotyped ways like rolling a ball or playing tickling
games. The issue may not be so much what the mother had done to her, but what
she did NOT have done. She may have had no experience of games that limited
teasing to a minimum (after all, teasing can enter any interaction, especially
tickling), nor of play styles that help the excited child return to a moderate
level of arousal. She may have seen other adults play in ways that brought the
child to a peak of agitation, followed by the adult’s losing interest and
turning away. In addition to all that, she may have valued personality
characteristics that included aggressiveness and recklessness, and may have
seen them as “manly”.
It might well be that we could be most helpful to
this mother and child by helping the mother learn better ways to play, rather than by
exploring her psychodynamics or condemning her as abusive. But, best of luck to
us for getting any funding for this project nowadays!
No comments:
Post a Comment