From time to time I see advertisements for a
residential treatment center known as CALO (www.caloteens.com),
or for programs apparently related to this model. The programs provide
residential treatment for children and teenagers who, the proprietors claim,
are difficult to parent because they suffer from Reactive Attachment Disorder.
RAD is said to make them lonely and miserable, unable to “bond” with others,
uncooperative, poor school achievers, etc., etc. As occurs all too often, these
proprietors note their belief that adoption even at birth is likely to result
in these undesirable outcomes.
Let’s have a look at one CALO website, where “our
proprietary treatment” is described at www.caloteens.com/message2.html.
I want to note first that “proprietary treatment” is a term generally reserved
for methods whose details are considered to be trade secrets, statements about
which are protected as commercial speech in the United States. Unlike
information about research-validated, evidence-based treatments, for which
details are easily available if you know where to look, proprietary treatments are
difficult and usually quite expensive to learn about-- as a rule, you have to sign up for workshops
or seminars or buy material from a suggested reading list sold only through the
proprietors.
Material at caloteens.com suggests that a major CALO
concern has to do with a rivalry with behavioral modification programs. As is
typical of non-evidence-based, commercially driven proprietary treatment
programs, the CALO discussion argues that behavior modification, which is seen
as a rival, replicates harmful situations that have already affected the
children, and that recovery from childhood problems must begin “with the heart”
and be followed later by behavior change. It is not stated with any clarity how
any “heart” changes can be detected before they are followed by behavior
change, and therefore it is far from clear how CALO’s claimed (but unlisted)
research basis could have been established.
The CALO website also stresses the need for
specialized treatment of childhood mental health problems, and notes that their
staff are specialists in treatment of attachment and trauma disorders, as
described and trained by groups like ATTACh and the Attachment & Trauma
Network. These comments are red flags for the possibility of two difficulties
often associated with proprietary treatments.
One is the assumption that some single factor, such as
attachment, is the single most important cause of a wide range of developmental
and emotional problems; like the bed of Procrustes, this assumption compresses
or stretches problems caused by combinations of biological and environmental
problems so that they “fit” the chosen bed—in this case, the attachment bed. As
has been pointed out by the British psychologists Woolgar and Scott, this sort
of single-factor explanation opens the door to choices that ignore not only
complex causes, but even simple factors that differ from the chosen cause.
A second red flag has to do with the assumption that
the details of a CALO program are of necessity essential for treatment.
However, serious work in clinical psychology has for years focused on general
or shared factors that contribute to good outcomes achieved through treatments
that are different in details. In some cases, such as EMDR, specific details
(like eye movement) may have nothing to do with positive outcomes, which
probably result from general helpful factors like empathic responses. The CALO claim
to uniqueness of its program is thus not likely to be a strong argument for
people with training in understanding therapeutic approaches—but it is quite
likely to appeal to worried parents.
However, let’s go on to my favorite bit of the CALO
website. This is the part about golden retriever therapy and the transfer of
“attachment lessons” learned from dogs, to human relationships. Kids in CALO
programs take care of dogs; they are said to “learn trust” from the dogs,
therefore to understand attachment, and therefore (with some additional,
undescribed help) to transfer the attachment they have learned from the dog to
a human being. This is quite an interesting idea, but one that makes a common
but mistaken assumption about emotional development, and also one that betrays
considerable confusion about how attachment works and what an attachment
relationship is.
The first issue here is one that I have often termed “ritual
reenactment”. The basic idea is that if certain events lead to a positive outcome
for infants and young children, those events, reenacted in some way in later
life, will recapitulate normal development and correct any problems that occurred
when they were wrongly experienced earlier on. This belief has appeared in many
forms from Sandor Ferenczi’s “babying” of patients to the methods of Frieda
Fromm-Reichmann as fictionalized in I Never
Promised You a Rose Garden to Nancy
Thomas’s insistence on bottle-feeding older children. None of these methods has
ever been shown to be effective, but somehow the thought of a “do-over”
continues to have a strong appeal to the public.
But-- suppose
that just for the sake of argument we accepted the idea of the “do-over”, would
caring for a golden retriever be a way to do this? A comparison of the ordinary
and the “treatment” situations says it would not. In typical early development,
a child is cared for by a consistent and responsive small group of adults. The
adults care for the infant physically,
but they also spend much time showing their positive feelings about him or her,
working toward communication of child to adult and adult to child, and enjoying
play and social interactions that bring pleasure to both adult and baby. The
outcome of these experiences is that the toddler stays close to the familiar
adult if anything is scary or distressing, can be comforted by the adult hen
distressed, and explores new things best if allowed to have contact with a
familiar person at will. (This set of behaviors has been summarized as “trust”
or as “attachment”, although those terms really apply to a hypothesized inner
state that guides the behavior. ) Well before school age, children put their
social experiences to work to build a set of ideas about how people interact socially,
sometimes called an internal working model of social relations (IWM). The IWM
continues to develop, sometimes along new lines, as the child grows and has new
social experiences.
How does that set of events compare to caring for a
dog? First of all, the roles are reversed. The human being acts as the “parent”
and the dog as the “child”. If the boy or girl does a good job of nurturing and
playing with the dog, the dog will develop trust in the boy or girl—but certainly
not an exclusive trust, especially if the dog is a very sociable golden
retriever. The CALO website says that the child learns empathy for the dog and
therefore becomes more empathic toward his or her parents, but it is far from
clear how either of these things could happen. If a child is a callous,
unemotional individual, in what way will doing the work of caring for a dog teach
or motivate empathic skills? And, if the child did become able to empathize
with a dog, read the dog’s signals, become aware of the dog’s usual needs, even
realize that any golden will convey that he needs yet another roast beef
sandwich because there really wasn’t any meat in the one you just gave him—how do
any of these skills relate to the more complex needs and messages of human
beings, the facial expressions, the body language, and all the other factors
that influence empathic responses? How
do any of these enter into the IWM’s further development? Indeed, if trust and
attachment were transferable, there would presumably be no attachment disorders
in adopted children, as all (according to the CALO website) must have been
attached to adults in the past, even at birth, so they ought to be able to hand
that attachment package over to a new caregiver, just as they are claimed to “transfer”
attachment from a dog to a human.
Since the relatively new developmental trauma disorder
fad came on the scene, I’ve been expecting to see fewer extravagant claims
about attachment, but it seems that CALO and similar groups are getting all the
juice they can out of the mythology of attachment. And, of course, therapy
dogs, emotional support animals, etc. are now in fashion, so why not bring in
the golden retrievers too?
Perhaps we’re lucky that they haven’t decided to
create attachment through pot-bellied pigs.