A reader’s recent comment referred to the tendency
shown by believers in the “Attachment Therapy” system to attribute children’s
behavioral difficulties to a lack of cause-and-effect thinking. At www.fromsurvivaltoserenity.com/2014/08/educating-about-rad.html,
a graphic is presented showing dozens of “symptoms” of Reactive Attachment
Disorder, few or none of them to be seen in the DSM-5 list of criteria for the
disorder. Among them, problems with cause and effect thinking are noted. A
document for teachers at www.attachmentnewengland.com/documents/educators/pdf
also refers to such problems, and claims that children with Reactive Attachment
Disorder cannot learn from behavior modification methods that use reinforcement
for desirable behavior. (The latter document warns parents against using
sarcasm, while providing a list of obviously sarcastic responses to children-- a point that raises questions about the sincerity
of the writer—and states that attachment begins prenatally, raising further
questions about the writer’s knowledge of the field.)
Strangely, however, these documents also stress the
belief that children with behavioral difficulties can manipulate, exploit, and
fool intelligent, well-trained, and experienced adults. How do they manage
this, one must ask, if they cannot associate cause (their own behavior or speech) with effect (the beliefs and behavior produced in the apparently hapless
adult)? For that matter, without some mastery of cause and effect associations,
how can they do anything at all in the way of self-care, schoolwork, household
chores, or play?
It does seem that just as these documents are not
really talking about Reactive Attachment Disorder when they use that term,
neither are they really talking about understanding cause and effect. Let’s
have a look at the development of cause and effect thinking first, then maybe I
can hazard some guesses about the real issues referred to by that name.
Learning about cause and effect is a gradual process,
but one which begins quite early and is slowed only by cognitive impairment.
Jean Piaget, the famous theorist of cognitive development, first described some
steps in understanding cause and effect almost a hundred years ago. He
suggested that between about 4 and 8 months of age babies begin to notice the
effect of things they do, like kicking their feet or making sounds. They
discover that sometimes their activities seem to make interesting events (like
an adult smiling at them) continue-- but
this little insight into cause and effect is only a beginning, because if the
interesting event stops, the baby gives up and does not try to make it start
again. From about 8 to 12 months, babies begin to put together single activities
so that they can look, reach, and grab, and use that combination to keep
interesting things going on. But, in Piaget’s theory (and many observations), a
much more important step waits until about 12 to 18 months, when babies catch
on to the fact that they can actually start interesting events by
themselves-- and they do this over and
over, by dropping and throwing objects and watching carefully to see what
happens (cereal splashes, cheese doesn’t, and they all get eaten by the dog). This
understanding of cause and effect is needed for a wide range of learned skills,
from pulling a stool to the table in order to reach something, to drinking
through a straw, to all forms of communication with other people and all
planning of actions.
It’s common for people to assume that Piaget was
taking a behaviorist position-- that
infants develop cognitively only because they are repeatedly gratified by what
happens when they exercise a new skill. That assumption is reflected in the
idea of the “bonding cycle” of alternate needs and gratifications, as claimed
by Attachment Therapy advocates. However, that is not at all what Piaget said. His theory (and recent work on cognitive
growth) is based on the idea that
cognitive development is driven from within and requires only ordinary
experiences that the baby himself produces. When the attachmentnewengland
document says “this [bonding] cycle promotes the development of cause and
effect thinking which is the basis of all problem solving”, it reveals a misunderstanding of how cognitive
development proceeds. Naturally, reasonable physical care is needed for
survival and healthy brain development, and social interaction is needed for
attachment and language learning, but experiences of care are not privileged
factors in the development of cause and effect thinking. To assume that they
are is simply a rationale for the use of non-evidence-based methods.
Of course, the understanding of cause and effect is
not complete at age 18 months. The more steps intervene Rube-Goldberg-like
between cause and effect, the harder the connection is to make. When someone
doesn’t understand how something works, their comprehension of specific causes
and effects will be limited. In the natural world, it is common for more than
one cause to produce an effect, for one cause to produce more than one effect,
and so on, and these situations make real understanding more difficult. In addition,
even adults are prone to fallacious reasoning about causes. They may accept
superstitious beliefs about spilling salt or having a black cat walk in front
of them-- a matter of confusing cause
and effect relationships. They may be swayed by fallacies about the order of
events and how that reveals cause and effect, so that they assume that when B
followed A, it must have been caused by A. They readily jump to the conclusion
that if two things are correlated, one must have caused the other. In the
Attachment Therapy- Nancy Thomas-Foster Cline belief system, they assume that
when adopted children have mood and behavior problems, the events surrounding
adoption must have caused the later problems.
Unless children have severe cognitive impairments,
we can expect them at school age to be somewhere between toddlers and adults in
their understanding of cause and effect. They know that they can make things
happen, but they are still likely to display fallacious reasoning about complicated
causes or those that are separated in time from their effects. Certainly they
are able to know when their actions bring about immediate approval or
disapproval from adults. As for responsiveness to behavior modification
techniques, there is no reason why they should not respond as well to a
properly-designed and implemented program as do all other living creatures
right down to flatworms.
What’s the problem, then, if there’s no actual
difficulty with understanding cause and effect? Might it not be that advocates
of Attachment Therapy beliefs are convinced that when they use poor behavior
management methods, those methods fail to be effective only because the
children are so bad? Using behavior modification
or similar management methods requires careful planning and depends strongly on
timely intervention. Research in this area showed many years ago that rewards
work best when they come very quickly after an desired behavior, and reprimands
or “consequences” for undesired behaviors work best if they occur in the middle
of the act, or even better, just as the child prepares to do it. There is no
reason to think that these rules would apply differently to children said to
have Reactive Attachment Disorder than to other beings.
It’s my guess that much of the issue of unwanted
behavior in “these children” (those considered to have attachment disorders by Attachment
Therapists) actually has to do with the idea that the children must never “win”
or “be in control”. The attachmentnewengland document recommends, per Nancy
Thomas, that an adult “establish eye contact with the child and ensure that the
child always looks up at the adult. The child with Reactive Attachment Disorder
dislikes eye contact and will try to avoid it except when he/she is lying or
trying to manipulate others. Avoid bending down to establish eye gaze with the
student…”. In other words, mutual gaze is required
because the child does not like it, and
he or she must be made to submit, ideally through an uncomfortable posture that
reminds the child of the adult’s size and power.
The document also insists that no explanations may
be given to a child who is “consequenced”. “When giving a consequence,
educators must stop themselves from
telling the child why a consequence is given. When a child doesn’t have cause
and effect thinking, he/she will never connect their inappropriate action with
the consequence no matter how many times the connection is explained. … The
child really doesn’t want to know why and just wants to argue… After the
consequence has been given, it is important for educators to let go of caring
about whether or not the consequence changes the child’s behavior, or has any
impact upon the child, or his/her actions…”.
It appears, then, that teachers are to abandon any
hope of establishing cause and effect thinking in their pupils; if a child
cannot make the connection with an explanation, surely he or she will not make
it without one. In addition, teachers
are apparently being advised to abandon their own capacity for cause and effect
thinking, and to ignore the outcome of their methods rather than to pay
attention to what works for them with a given child. Rather than modeling the
thinking behavior they should want, they are to adhere rigidly to what they are
told, and to fall into their places in this authoritarian system just as the
children are to do.
This is not about cause and effect thinking, any
more than it’s about attachment or even Reactive Attachment Disorder as the
evidence shows it to be. It’s all about obedience and punishment. Cotton Mather
would recognize it easily.