As most readers will know, the American Psychiatric
Association periodically publishes a new edition of the Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders. Years of
work between editions contribute to removal or addition of diagnostic
categories, and revision of criteria for particular diagnoses. The differences
between older and newer standards make it very clear that mental illnesses-- while very real-- are understood through construction and
re-construction of ideas about mood and behavior. Childhood mental illness is
particularly subject to construction, because most of our thoughts about it
have to do with what we adults expect of children, not how children expect or
want to feel.
There is now
a new edition of the Diagnostic and
statistical manual of mental disorders. Known as DSM-V, it differs in a
number of ways from the previous edition, DSM-IV-TR, which came out in 2000. It’s
especially interesting to see what changes have been made in the diagnosis of
Reactive Attachment Disorder, a childhood emotional problem. DSM-IV-TR (and two
editions before that) described two variations in which RAD could appear. One
of them, the inhibited form, involved clinging to caregivers and being
unusually shy with strangers. The other, disinhibited form, was one in which
toddlers and preschoolers did not seem to have the usual strong preferences for
familiar people that most children their age show. They did not seem especially
interested in or dependent on familiar adults, and were quite happy to interact
with unfamiliar people. Both forms were associated with neglectful or abusive
early parenting and with frequent changes of caregivers. The problem behaviors
appeared before the children were five years old.
How severe are the problem behaviors? There has been
enough research to show that they are not very severe (although of course any
child may have multiple disorders, some associated with severe mood and
behavior problems). Basically, no significant association has been shown
between the “clingy” type and externalizing problems like aggressive behavior.
Neither has there been a significant association between the “too friendly”
category and aggression. The “too friendly” category is moderately associated
with inattention and hyperactivity (see http://sti.mimhtraining.com).
Thus, as is the case for some other difficulties of
early development, these two versions of RAD were not so much problematic in themselves
as in the developmental trajectories they shaped. That is, except for the
possibility of abduction, there are no immediate dangers associated either with
being “too friendly” or “too clingy”. However, the clingy child misses many
opportunities to learn and develop through contacts with other people, and may
find school too anxiety-producing to benefit from attendance, thus getting
farther and farther behind in social and intellectual skills, compared to more
typical children. The disinhibited, “too friendly” child may miss out on
learning about family values and attitudes because of his or her willingness to
pay as much attention to outsiders as to familiar caregivers. Over the years of
development, either of these extremes will probably produce less than ideal
social and intellectual development--
although it’s possible that new experiences may help an individual child
move in the direction of typical development.
As DSM-V was prepared, committee members working on
diagnostic categories consulted professional discussions of diagnostic
approaches and examined empirical work--
in the case of RAD, research on the characteristics of children
diagnosed with RAD. They received comments from interested members of the
public as well as considering diagnostic categories used outside the U.S. The
comments are interesting to examine, as in some cases they were concerned with
issues outside the matters the DSM committees focused on. For example, at http://blog.radzebra.org/?p=12, the
author Julie Beem stated her objection to a proposed change in the RAD category,
the addition of a diagnosis called Disinihibited Social Engagement Disorder: “The criterion…’persistent harsh punishment or
other types of grossly inept parenting’ is alarming… The danger of using ‘grossly
inept parenting’ as a criterion is the blame it places on whoever is currently
parenting the child. Grossly inept parenting is difficult to define and the
words are emotionally loaded. This criterion could actually make it harder for
children to be correctly treated, served, or diagnosed because of the stigma of
bad parenting. It could lead to the removal of children from safe, loving homes
where they are exhibiting these symptoms with their new, appropriate caregivers.”
Ms. Beem, like a number of commenters, seems to have missed the point that DSM
categories are supposed to be based on systematic research, rather than on the
possible impact on caregivers of naïve application of these categories; she may
also have been concerned about whether some of the methods encouraged by
radzebra.org could themselves be classed as grossly inept parenting. Like some
others (see http://center4familydevelopment.blogspot.com),
Ms. Beem wanted the DSM committee to add
a new category, Developmental Trauma Disorder, rather than altering the RAD
criteria, but although this proposed diagnosis has received much discussion in
certain quarters, the research evidence to support its inclusion does not
exist.
What did DSM-V actually do in its construction of Reactive
Attachment Disorder? The final decision was to split the previously-existing
category into two separate diagnoses. Reactive Attachment Disorder is now
defined as a lack of or incomplete formation of preferred attachments to familiar
people, with a dampening of positive affect that resembles internalizing
disorders (for example, anxiety). Disinhibited Social Engagement Disorder is
more like ADHD and may occur in children who have clearly formed, even secure, attachments.
This fact suggests strongly that treatment for the second disorder need not
focus on attachment (although any mental health intervention has a basis in
relationship-building).
Has your child been diagnosed with RAD by someone
who claims that the untreated disorder will
lead to serial killing? Do you have an inattentive child who has been
said to have an attachment disorder? It’s
time for a new diagnosis by someone who understands and uses evidence-based
categories.