Of all the posts I’ve ever done on this blog, the
ones most likely to be commented on have been those dealing with the claim that
spanking children causes them to be violently aggressive, either then or in
later life. This idea received much elaboration from the late Alice Miller, a
strongly psychoanalytically-oriented scholar and popular author, and her fans
continue to pursue the same way of thinking. (I must say I give credit to both
Miller and her followers for their wish to find ways to moderate human
violence-- at the same time that I am
compelled to say that they are wrong in their understanding of aggressive
behavior.)
I am bringing up this topic again today because of
an interesting issue of the magazine Science,
the house organ of the American Association for the Advancement of Science.
This issue (18 May 2012) focuses on the topic of human conflict. In a special
section, authors discuss the aggressive behavior of our pre-human ancestors,
issues about ethnic and cultural conflict and racism, the evolution of empathy,
the treatment of PTSD among veterans of Iraq and Afghanistan, and the
possibility of life without war as modeled by societies in the past and
present. Does anyone notice what topic is not
here? That’s right-- there is no
discussion of the origins of human aggression in childhood experience. A group
of authors examining various aspects of human conflict did not look to spanking
as a cause of violence. Instead, they assumed that aggressive behavior is one
aspect of human life, just as it is a natural aspect for all our primate
relatives.
Humans are able to be aggressive in various ways, as
is the case for all mammals, however peaceful and long-suffering they may be.
Like other mammals, humans can show aggression impulsively and under the
motivation of anger or fear. They can also show instrumental aggression, by harming or distressing other creatures
without being angry or afraid, but instead motivated by the wish to create a
desirable outcome. Organized societies use instrumental aggression for various
reasons. They appoint soldiers to aggress against other people with whom they
may not be angry (as Muhammad Ali famously said when refusing induction into
the army, “I ain’t got nothing against them Viet Congs”) but who may pose some
imminent or remote threat to the group. They choose police officers to arrest
and incarcerate people who have harmed or threatened others, usually not the
police themselves. They employ judges to decide what punishment should be
received by convicted criminals who generally are no threat to the judges.
Spanking or other physical punishments of children
can be matters of impulsive, angry, or fearful emotionally-driven
behavior-- and when they are of this
type, they may or may not cause serious physical harm. They may also be
instrumental aggression, designed to bring about a desired result such as
changes in child behavior. Followers of Alice Miller tend to emphasize
repressed motives of pleasure in causing pain to a child-- and obviously these can be present and guide
the adult’s actions-- but it is also
possible that physical punishment is considered by the adult as simply a means
to an end, no more associated with anger or fear than is the prick of the vaccination
needle intended to protect the child from disease.
But if everyone shares a human capacity to behave
aggressively, why is it that some are so much more aggressive than others? In a
recent paper, the well-known developmental psychologist Theodore Beauchaine
discussed how genetic predispositions interact with family behavior to create problem
outcomes in the form of externalizing disorders-- emotional disturbances that lead to unjustified
attacks on other people. (Beauchaine’s paper, “Physiological markers of emotion
and behavior dysregulation in externalizing psychopathology”, is in a monograph
of the Society for Research on Child Development, Physiological measures of emotion from a developmental perspective:
State of the science, 2012; edited by T.A. Dennis, K.A. Buss, and P.D. Hastings.)
Beauchaine suggested that neither a genetic tendency toward impulsiveness nor
poor family experiences created a strong tendency toward delinquent or
aggressive behavior-- but a combination
of the two is necessary before externalizing problems are likely to develop.
Children who are constitutionally impulsive learn in their families good or poor
ways to regulate themselves emotionally, and those whose emotional regulation
is poor are likely to be inappropriately aggressive as they suffer for long
periods from sadness, fear, or anger that they have not learned to cope with.
Beauchaine’s work shows how other issues connected
with physical punishment could be causes of later aggressive behavior-- parents who rely primarily on spanking to
control child behavior may not supply any help in learning emotional
regulation, whereas those who spank less or not at all may offer guidance in emotional regulation.
For example, the non-spankers may do more talking about emotion, offer words to
describe emotions, and point out how they or others are made to feel if the
child misbehaves. The child in turn can use self-talk about emotion to reduce
persistent and uncomfortable negative feelings—a strategy that would not be
learned in a family that depended on physical punishment for discipline.
Following this line of thought, we seem to arrive at
the idea that basic human characteristics cause aggression, but that
appropriate family experiences can modulate aggressive tendencies and guide
them along appropriate pathways. The issue may not be whether or not to spank
children, but instead what positive and helpful experiences children need in
order to develop good self-regulation and control over aggressive impulses.
Prohibiting spanking may not be an effective strategy after all. Our efforts may best be put into offering parents
guidance about supporting their children’s development of emotional regulation.